Index/Search  Leonardo On-Line  About Leonardo  Whats New








Reviewer Biography

Books

Compact Disks

Events/Exhibitions

Film/Video

Metaphors of Memory: A history of ideas about the mind
By Douwe Draaisma
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. 2000.
241 pp. Illus. b/w. ISBN 0-521-65024-0, $29.95

Reviewed by: Robert Pepperell
School of Art. Media and Design
University of Wales College, Newport
Caerleon Campus
Newport NP18 3YH

pepperell@ntlworld.com

Early in 'Metaphors of Memory' we are reminded that two of the most productive scientific thinkers of the twentieth century, Sigmund Freud and Albert Einstein, used metaphors prolifically both to generate hypotheses and propagate their theories. Grand science, it seems, frequently turns to metaphor when trying to illuminate the unknown, thereby providing one of the few examples of fruitful convergence between poetics, artistic vision and hard scientific progress. Indeed Freud, quoted here in correspondence with Ferenczi, defines scientific creativity as the interplay between "daringly playful fantasy and relentlessly realistic criticism" (p. 8), a combination of the imaginative subject with objective materialism which is sometimes lacking in contemporary reductive science.

Moving from a summary of some established theories about metaphor in the introduction to a heuristic analysis of Freud's famous wax-based 'Mystic Writing-Pad' in chapter one, Draaisma begins to reveal the extent of our reliance on metaphor when describing the human mind; for it is apparent from the subtitle of this book that one cannot understand memory without provoking some theory of the mind as a whole. The materials employed by the Mystic Writing-Pad neatly link us back, through Edison's Phonographic wax drum with its 'memory of sound', to some of the earliest known analogies of memory, considered in chapter two, which also relied on the capacity of wax to take an impression. Plato and Aristotle both developed models of mental reception and retrieval based on the wax tablets then in common use and these, in turn, led to various analogies of memory as writing.

Advancing through the centuries, and various mnemonic comparisons, Draaisma reminds us of the fact that the imaginative use of one thing to enlighten our understanding of another is often closely allied to our experience of the currently available technology - a point that has been made by others before, including John Searle. What makes this book worthwhile, however, is the depth and breadth of the investigation into the historical relationship between technology and models of the mind as well as the surprising nature of some of the metaphors that have latterly been employed. Using clear, measured prose and a pacy narrative flow, Draaisma recounts that memory has been variously compared to abbeys and cathedrals, the aeolian harp, aviaries, theatres with stages and wings, storehouses and warehouses, phosphorescent substances, clockwork mechanisms, the telephone exchange, railway networks, the camera obscura and photography, the phonograph and cinematograph, and the more recently familiar computers systems and holographs. Around this historical data Draaisma constructs an elegant series of arguments concerning the wider social and philosophical imperatives driving scientists and thinkers to view the mind in certain lights. Frequently these arguments also illuminate the developmental processes of the very technologies that, in turn, inspired the metaphors. For example, in the section dealing with "the brain as a conscious phonograph" (p. 90) it is suggested that the excitement for nineteenth century witnesses of hearing recorded sound played back by the phonograph was less its capacity for storage than the illusion it created of a self-aware machine. Hence in Edison's early demonstrations he gets the machine to enquire after the health of journalists from Scientific American as if it were some low fidelity homunculus (p. 88).

Despite his obvious enthusiasm for the many metaphors he discusses, Draaisma is also keen to expose their flaws and weaknesses, as many contemporary commentators have been. The holographic metaphor, discussed at length in chapter seven, was initially greeted with some relief by psychologists who had no other physical models for understanding what had been experimentally verified about the functionality of the brain (p. 174). By the mid-twentieth century it was becoming clear that brain functionality is both localised (as Broca had established in the nineteenth century) and global (as Lashley had observed in rats with progressive cortex lesions). Yet, whilst the holistic nature of the holographic plate seemed to offer an ideal model of a physical process that could express such contradictory behaviour (since, unlike the photograph, in the hologram the localised image is distributed across the whole plate), it has ultimately proved of limited heuristic value. In quantifying the decline in the number of references and citations of papers which offer a holographic analogy to memory over the last 30 years, Draaisma argues that when examined closely the comparison with actual human mental processes is very slight, and in fact in many ways misleading. Still, it's worth remembering that: "To profit from a metaphor, we must avoid too literal use of it" (p. 177) whilst accepting that, in many ways, the recourse to metaphor is an admission of a failure of explanation.

Appropriate to current debates in consciousness studies, cognitive psychology and artificial intelligence research, much of the latter half of the book is devoted to the various digital and computer models of mind, with particular attention paid to the tension between the symbolic and the connectionist approaches. Perhaps this is the least interesting territory, being well catered for in numerous other publications, although valuable here as a clear introduction to the histories and debates.

Together Douwe Draaisma and Cambridge University Press have produced a book of high quality. Extensively referenced and illustrated, impeccably rendered from Dutch to English and fully indexed, this work transcends what could be the narrow discipline of the history of psychology (in which the author is a lecturer at the University of Groningen). It becomes instead a solid textbook on the broader history of the understanding of the human mind that could serve as reading material for any number of courses. However, it should be ackowledged that this is an exclusively western European history of mind, and to that extent not entirely comprehensive. Finally, I should point out that it's simply a good book to read!

top







Updated 5 December 2001.




Contact LDR: ldr@leonardo.org

Contact Leonardo: isast@leonardo.info


copyright © 2001 ISAST